(2011) showed that the RIM1 and RIM2 PDZ domains directly bind to

(2011) showed that the RIM1 and RIM2 PDZ domains directly bind to the C-terminal sequences of the P/Q- and N-type Ca2+ channel α subunits and that the PDZ domain was required to rescue the decreased Ca2+ transients and the abnormal Ca2+ channel localization. In addition, the proline-rich domain of RIM1/2 was also necessary for the rescue, suggesting that a tripartite interaction between RIM1/2, RIM-BPs (which bind to the proline-rich

domain of RIMs; Hibino et al., 2002) and the Ca2+ channel α subunits is critical for holding Ca2+ channel at the active zone. This, together with the present work, identifies RIM1/2 as a presynaptic scaffolding proteins with a clear role in maintaining a high Ca2+ channel density at the presynaptic active zone. Previous work

has suggested a role for Bruchpilot, a Drosophila gene related to ELKS/CAST proteins, in maintaining presynaptic Ca2+ selleck chemicals channel density and structural entirety at Drosophila presynaptic active zones ( Kittel et al., 2006). Conditional removal of RIM1/2 did not significantly change the relative contribution of P/Q-type and N-type channels to the total presynaptic Ca2+ current, despite a strongly reduced total Ca2+ current. Anti-infection Compound Library This finding is different to the situation in P/Q-type (α1A subunit) KO mice (Jun et al., 1999), in which a strong compensatory upregulation of presynaptic N-type channels was observed at the calyx of Held (Inchauspe et al., 2004 and Ishikawa et al., 2005). Thus, in the absence of RIM1/2, N-type Ca2+ channels are not capable of compensating for the missing P/Q-type channels; therefore, the absence of RIM1/2 probably affects both Ca2+ channel α subunits equally. The calyx synapse has been an ideal preparation to functionally define a fast and a slow subpool of the readily releasable pool Org 27569 (Sakaba and Neher, 2001, Wölfel et al., 2007 and Wadel et al., 2007; see Neher, 2006 for a review). Here, we find that conditional removal of all major RIM isoforms leads to a strong, ∼70% decrease of the readily releasable pool size as defined by various types of pool-depleting Ca2+ stimuli (Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5).

Importantly, we found a very similar decrease of the number of docked vesicles at the active zone (by ∼70%; Figure 6), demonstrating a genetic manipulation that leads to a parallel decrease in the number of docked vesicles and of the readily releasable pool size determined functionally. Thus, at the calyx of Held, the amount of vesicle docking seems to determine the size of the readily releasable pool. This conclusion is further supported by the reasonable quantitative agreement between the number of docked vesicles on the one hand (∼seven docked vesicle per active zone ∗ 500 ≈3500; assuming that a calyx has ∼500 active zones; Sätzler et al., 2002 and Taschenberger et al., 2002), and the sum of FRP and SRP vesicles on the other hand (∼3000–3500; see Figure 5).

, 2008; Mizuseki et al , 2009, 2011) Spike sorting was carried o

, 2008; Mizuseki et al., 2009, 2011). Spike sorting was carried out offline from the digitally high-pass filtered (0.8–5 kHz) data using an automatic clustering Alpelisib price algorithm (http://klustakwik.sourceforge.net) (Mizuseki et al., 2009). Principal cells and interneurons were separated on the basis of their autocorrelograms, combination of trough-to-peak latency,

and the asymmetry index of the filtered spike waveform, bursting properties, and mean firing rates (Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Two major comparisons of firing patterns and LFP were used. First, changes across sleep were defined as differences between the first and the last non-REM episodes in a sleep session. Second, the duration of REM and non-REM episodes was normalized (100% each epoch) and divided into equal normalized thirds. Changes within the episodes were then analyzed by comparing the first and the last thirds of each REM and non-REM episode. Third, changes within REM episodes refer to differences between the first and the last thirds of each REM. Ripple events were detected during nontheta periods from the band-pass filtered (120–250 Hz) trace by defining periods during which ripple power is continuously greater than mean 2 SD, and peak of power in the periods was greater than mean 3 SD of ripple power. Three approaches were used

to characterize firing patterns associated with ripples. (1) Within-ripple firing rate: all www.selleckchem.com/products/at13387.html ripples within a given epoch E were concatenated. Within-ripple firing rate for almost each cell C is defined as the number of spikes detected in the concatenated ripple epochs divided by the total concatenated time ( Figure 1Bvii). (2) Ripple participation: for each cell C, ripple participation is the percentage of ripples in

E in which C fired at least one spike ( Figures 1Bix and 2B, bottom, left). (3) Ripple participant firing rate: for each pyramidal cell C, only those ripples in a given epoch E in which C fired at least one spike were concatenated. Ripples in which C fired no spikes were excluded. Ripple participant firing rate was the total number of spikes divided by the total time in the concatenated subset of ripples ( Figure 1B). These methods were designed to disambiguate ripple-related firing rate changes due to increased spiking of the neuron C in the same number of ripples in different sleep episodes from increased participation of the neuron in more ripple events without changing the firing rate within individual ripple participation events. The relationship between LFP and firing patterns were examined using spectral methods. Further details about the experimental techniques are available in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures. We thank Mariano Belluscio, Adrien Peryache, and Richard W. Tsien for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the International Human Frontiers Science Program Organization, the U.S.

, 1995, Herberholz et al , 2002 and DeVries et al , 2002); (3) la

, 1995, Herberholz et al., 2002 and DeVries et al., 2002); (3) lateral excitation promotes the coordinated activity of a population of CEs; and (4) the coordinated activity likely increases efficacy of auditory input for the initiation of an escape response. These events are likely enhanced by electrical rectification, which favors the spread of currents from the M-cell lateral dendrite toward the presynaptic CEs. That is, because dendritic

currents would encounter a lower resistance to spread across these junctions than those generated presynaptically, electrical rectification favors the retrograde transmission of dendritic Selleck RO4929097 signals, counteracting the leak of currents toward the soma, following a pathway of low resistance (Figure 6). Moreover, given that coupling increases

with presynaptic depolarization, the voltage dependence of electrical coupling we describe here acts as a “coincidence detector,” promoting the recruitment of CEs that are already depolarized, such as during the invasion of an incoming action potential, whose depolarization (because of cable properties) travels several nodes ahead without reaching threshold (Figure 6). Thus, although differences in input resistance significantly contribute to the asymmetry of electrical transmission between these cells, rectification plays a critical functional role by directing currents toward the presynaptic http://www.selleckchem.com/products/z-vad-fmk.html endings. A generalized perception is that each side in most GJ plaques represents the mirror image of the other, as its formation requires the symmetric arrangement of hemichannels. This view, however, is changing with the recognition that connexins associate with a variety of proteins,

resulting mafosfamide in the formation of macromolecular complexes (Hervé et al., 2012). Furthermore, electrical synapses have been shown to be dynamic structures, where connexins actively turnover (Flores et al., 2012) and exhibit activity-dependent regulation of their coupling strength (Yang et al., 1990, Pereda and Faber, 1996, Landisman and Connors, 2005, Cachope et al., 2007 and Haas et al., 2011). These properties suggest that each side in a GJ plaque must be supported by a scaffold structure, similar to postsynaptic densities at chemical synapses (Kennedy, 2000). While the detailed composition of this scaffold is largely unknown, several molecules interact with Cx36 (Li et al., 2004, Li et al., 2009, Burr et al., 2005, Ciolofan et al., 2006 and Alev et al., 2008) and its teleost homologs (Flores et al., 2008 and Flores et al., 2010). Thus, molecular diversity in electrical synapses might not only be endowed by the connexins present but also potentially by differences in the ensemble of scaffold and regulatory molecules associated with each side of the gap junctions that form these synapses, which could be an additional means of creating molecular asymmetry, impacting on the functional properties of these channels.

To reveal the mechanism governing the neurovascular congruency in

To reveal the mechanism governing the neurovascular congruency in the whisker pad system, we first considered the contributions from the target tissues on their coordinated patterning. Emerging evidence suggests that the neural and vascular systems share many similar mechanisms and molecular cues to regulate their development (Adams and Eichmann, 2010, Carmeliet and Tessier-Lavigne, 2005 and Gelfand et al., 2009). Therefore, we next asked whether guidance cues could be found in the whisker follicle, and whether these cues could

function as an organizing signal to coordinately pattern the nerves and vessels into the congruent double ring structure. We first performed expression screening of known guidance molecules in the TG and whisker targets, and found a striking complementary expression pattern of Sema3e and its receptor Plxnd1 in these KU 55933 areas ( Figures 3A–3H). Moreover, the spatiotemporal expression of these molecules coincides with the developmental profile of whisker follicles. Plxnd1 mRNA was expressed at very low levels in the TG neurons at an early stage (E12.5), when axons arrive at the whisker target ( Figure 3A) ( van der Zwaag et al., 2002). Its expression level is significantly increased by E14.5, when trigeminal axons and blood vessels begin to organize the double ring structure

in the whisker follicles ( Figure 3B) and continues through E16.5 and E18.5, when target innervation is refined and a clear double ring structure is apparent ( Figure 3C; Vorinostat cost data not shown). In a manner similar to the temporal profile of Plxnd1 expression in the TG, Sema3e is not expressed at E12.5 in the whisker follicles ( Figure 3E). too Sema3e mRNA is found in the mesenchymal sheath surrounding the hair follicles starting

at E14.5 and continuing to E16.5 and E18.5 ( Figures 3F–3H). To characterize the precise location of Sema3e within the follicle, we performed immunostaining of axons after Sema3e in situ hybridization (ISH) on the same tissue sections and demonstrated that Sema3e was expressed inside the nerve ring ( Figures 3I and 3J). Finally, to examine whether Plxnd1 is also expressed in the outer blood vessel ring, we performed double fluorescence ISH of Plxnd1 and the endothelial cell marker Flk-1. As shown in Figure 3L, mRNA expression of Plxnd1 and Flk-1 completely overlaps within the vessel ring. Together, these expression data suggest that, in the whisker pad, Sema3E expression surrounds the hair follicle ( Figures 3K and 3M), while Plexin-D1 is expressed in the blood vessels and may also expressed in the innervating trigeminal axons based on its mRNA expression in the TG ( Figure 3M). This complementary expression pattern in the target area suggests that Sema3E-Plexin-D1 signaling plays a role in coordinating trigeminal target innervation and the formation of blood vessels into the double ring structure.

“Lateral” inhibition could occur if adjacent domains of sensory c

“Lateral” inhibition could occur if adjacent domains of sensory cortex (such as orientation columns within cat visual cortex or whisker maps in rodent barrel cortex) are tuned to different stimulus features—and inhibition in one cortical

subregion can be influenced by neighboring domains. While the necessary circuits for such lateral inhibitory interactions exist in cortex (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010), determining their exact spatial extent and impact on sensory processing will require more work. Furthermore, in the visual, auditory, and olfactory cortices of rodents, stimulus selective responses selleck inhibitor occur despite the fact that cells tuned to particular stimulus features are spatially intermingled in a “salt and pepper” organization

(Ohki et al., 2005, Rothschild et al., 2010 and Stettler and Axel, 2009). A less literal form of lateral inhibition that does not require a two dimensional spatial mapping of stimulus features still applies to cortical tuning: namely, that synaptic excitation to a preferred stimulus Cytoskeletal Signaling inhibitor roughly shapes the tuning of a cell’s spike output and that tuning is further sharpened by robust synaptic inhibition in response to nonpreferred stimuli (Priebe and Ferster, 2008). This notion, however, has been challenged by intracellular recording studies in several cortical regions showing that in individual neurons the stimuli that generate the strongest excitation (preferred stimuli) can be the same as those generating the strongest inhibition (Figures 2A and 3B; Anderson et al., 2000, Liu et al., 2011, Mariño et al., 2005, Martinez et al., 2002, Tan et al., 2004, Tan et al., 2011, Wehr and Zador, 2003, Wilent and Contreras, 2005, Wu et al., 2008 and Zhang et al., 2003, but see Monier et al., 2003). Furthermore, Thymidine kinase as the stimulus gradually changes away from the preferred feature, both excitation and inhibition

decrease. In other words the tuning curves for excitation and for inhibition show considerable overlap. How then could inhibition sharpen the tuning of cortical neurons to the preferred stimuli? This can happen in several ways. First, it is important to note that the tuning curve determined through the spike output of a neuron is not equal to the tuning curve determined by recording the membrane potential of that neuron. Because only the strongest excitatory input received by a neuron sufficiently depolarizes the membrane to reach threshold for spike generation, (i.e., the “tip” of the tuning curve of the membrane potential), the spike output of the neuron is more sharply tuned than the underlying membrane potential (Figure 4), a phenomenon appropriately called “iceberg effect” (Carandini and Ferster, 2000 and Rose and Blakemore, 1974). In other words, the non-linearity of spike rate versus membrane potential sharpens the tuning of a neuron.

In particular, functional CD39 and CD73 expressed by exosomes are

In particular, functional CD39 and CD73 expressed by exosomes are capable of dephosphorylating exogenous ATP and 5′AMP to form adenosine, thus contributing to rise

the adenosine levels within the microenvironment [53]. Tumor-derived exosomes can modulate other crucial components of the immune response, impacting on the functional properties of innate immunity. As an example, exosomes derived from human melanoma and colorectal carcinoma cell lines were shown to impair the capacity of circulating CD14+ monocytes to differentiate into functional dendritic cells (DC) and to skew them toward the differentiation into immunosuppressive selleck chemicals elements highly resembling the well-known population of myeloid-derived suppressor cells (MDSC) [54]. The hallmarks of this in vitro-induced new subset of MDSC were represented by the retention of CD14+ expression with concomitant low or absent levels of HLA-DR, together with the ability to inhibit T cell proliferation and function mostly through the release of TGFβ1 [55]. Interestingly, cells echoing this phenotype could be detected by our group in the peripheral blood of advanced melanoma patients; in fact, a significant

expansion of CD11b + CD14+HLA-DR−/low TGFβ-secreting cells, Cabozantinib datasheet was found in the peripheral blood of stage IV melanoma patients with respect to healthy donors, in association with a reduced ability to mount CD8+ T cell-mediated immune response upon vaccine administration [56]. Interestingly, the frequency of CD14+HLA-DR−/low TGFβ-secreting cells is increased already in peripheral blood of IIb–IIIc stage melanoma patients, suggesting that systemic MDSC expansion is an early event in this type of cancer, in contrast to regulatory T (Treg) cells, whose expansion is detectable only in advanced disease (P. Filipazzi, personal communication). These findings led to the hypothesis that

melanoma exosomes might be involved in driving MDSC expansion by possibly accumulating in the bone marrow, where they might influence myelopoiesis toward the differentiation of immunosuppressive pro-tumorigenic cell subsets [57]. CD14+HLA-DR−/low Dipeptidyl peptidase MDSC have also been found in peripheral blood of patients affected by other types of cancer, including hepatocellular carcinoma [58], bladder cancer [59] and multiple myeloma [60]. In these latter studies a direct link between tumor exosomes and the generation of monocyte-derived MDSC has not been investigated. However, we cannot exclude that exosomes secreted by tumor cells might contribute to this phenomenon. The interaction between the cellular immune system and cancer-derived exosomes can also directly support Treg expansion as well as their suppressive functions. In a recent study, Szajnik et al.

Systemic administration of the GABAB receptor agonist GBL induces

Systemic administration of the GABAB receptor agonist GBL induces experimental absence seizures in rodents (Ishige et al., DAPT supplier 1996). Our results demonstrated that the lack

of CaV2.3 channels in mice resulted in a marked decrement in the duration and power of GBL-induced 3–4 Hz SWDs, the hallmark of absence seizures. A pharmacological blockade of CaV2.3 channels in the RT also reduced the susceptibility of the mouse to GBL-induced 3–4 Hz SWDs, consistent with the results with the CaV2.3−/− mice. These results are consistent with a previous report that revealed a close correlation between SWDs on EEGs and rhythmic burst discharges of RT neurons on intracellular recordings observed in the genetic absence epileptic rat from Strasbourg (GAERS) model animals ( Slaght et al., 2002). Correspondingly, the preservation of rhythms in a deafferented RT leads Steriade et al. (1987) to propose that the RT is the see more generator of rhythmicity during EEG synchronizations. Our results in vitro as well as in vivo using genetic and pharmacological tools suggest that CaV2.3 channels are critical for the rhythmic burst discharges of RT neurons that in turn may maintain thalamocortical rhythms ( Llinas and Steriade, 2006 and Steriade et al., 1993). On the other

hand, we note that the tonic firing activity of the RT neurons is reduced in the mutant, as shown by the reduced responses to depolarizing inputs (Figure 6). Therefore, it is formally feasible to suppose that the reduced excitability of the thalamocortical Calpain network rendered by the mutation contributes to the decreased sensitivity of the mutant mice to GBL-induced seizure responses. Absence seizures are associated with EEG recordings of bilaterally synchronous SWDs. Here, we obtained simultaneous recordings of monopolar (Kim et al., 2001) and bipolar (subtraction method) EEGs (Weiergraber et al., 2008) in parallel from the same mice. However, only the monopolar data were included in the analysis because only this method of EEG recording yielded bilaterally synchronous SWDs with robust amplitudes, whereas bipolar recordings did not (∼10-fold

smaller), probably due to cancellation of the hemispherically symmetrical signals inherent to absence seizures (Figure S6). For this reason our findings may not be directly comparable to the bipolar EEG data previously reported for CaV2.3−/− mice ( Weiergraber et al., 2008). Patch-clamp and EEG recordings provide compelling evidence that CaV2.3 channels play a key role in the generation of rhythmic burst discharges of RT neurons and thalamocortical oscillations related to absence seizures. Moreover, it is known that LVA Ca2+ channels play an important role in absence seizures and sleep-related oscillations of the thalamocortical network ( Cheong et al., 2009, Cueni et al., 2008 and Kim et al., 2001). Taken together, understanding the functional consequences of modulation of HVA as well as LVA ( Shin, 2006 and Shin et al.

6 and 0 4 repeated in a later block but that those with 0 3 and 0

6 and 0.4 repeated in a later block but that those with 0.3 and 0.7 performance did not (see Figure 1B). Because the estimated probabilities for asset price increases fluctuated primarily between 0.25 and 0.75 (see Figure 2B), agent performance seldom reached unreasonably high or low levels given the predictability of the asset. Figure 1B summarizes the agent configuration and parameters used in the experiment. For the human agents, we used male faces of the same approximate age to minimize any potential inferences of ability based on age or gender-related

cues. Assignment of specific faces and fractal images to agent predictions was pseudorandomly determined and counterbalanced across subjects. Importantly, at the beginning of the experiment, subjects were told that the

asset performance evolved over time but were not given the details of the specific process. In addition, they PD0325901 order were told that real people and computerized algorithms programmed by the experimenters to track the asset had previously made predictions about whether the asset would increase or decrease in value and that those constituted the predictions that they would bet on. They were also informed that the identities of the faces displayed did not correspond to the actual people who had made the prior predictions. Finally, they were told that people agents were selected such that they differed in their abilities to track the asset, and likewise for algorithms. We compared the extent to which various models could learn more account for the subjects’ behavior when predicting the agent’s ability and the performance of the assets. Except for the Full Model, these models consisted of two separable components: a model for the performance

of the asset, and a model of the agent’s ability. These models use the history of observed evidence to update beliefs about the agents’ abilities and about the state of the asset. The model of how subjects learn the probability of asset price changes is based on previous work on Bayesian reward learning (Behrens Terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase et al., 2007, Behrens et al., 2008 and Boorman et al., 2011). A detailed description of this model and its estimation is provided in the Supplemental Information, as well as in the supplemental tables and figures of these studies; for example, Behrens et al. (2007). We considered four distinct but natural classes of behavioral models. We refer to the classes as the full model, pure evidence model, the pure simulation model, and the sequential model. A formal description of the full model is provided in the Supplemental Information. Let qt denote the probability that the asset goes up at time t, according to the subject’s beliefs at the time. The remaining models have some common properties, which we discuss first. Inferences about agent expertise are made based on the performance of the agent’s guesses. Let gt denote the subject’s belief about the quality of the guess made by the agent presented at time t.

The number of cannabis users increases with age as does the frequ

The number of cannabis users increases with age as does the frequency of use. Cannabis users did not differ from non-users with respect to SES (t(1447) = −.9, p = .387), gender (χ2 (1) = 1.1, p = .289), familial vulnerability for internalizing (t(1447) = −.4, p = .705) and externalizing behaviour (t(1447) = −1.8, p = .071). Cannabis users and non-users differed significantly with respect to alcohol use at T2 (χ2 (1) = 90.3, p < .001), alcohol use at T3 (χ2 (1) = 95.0, p < .001), tobacco use at T2 (χ2 (1) = 137.3, p < .001)

and tobacco use EGFR inhibitor at T3 χ2 (1) = 346.8, p < .001), with cannabis users using alcohol and tobacco more often than non-users (57.8% vs. 31.2% reported monthly alcohol use at T2; percentages for T3: 94.0% vs. 70.7%; 19.8% vs. 2.2% reported weekly tobacco use at T2; percentages for T3: 57.4% vs. 11.1%). Tobacco and alcohol use were also related to both internalizing and externalizing behaviour and therefore included as covariates in subsequent find protocol path analysis (for detailed information, see Table 2). Factor loadings of the indicators of the latent variables of internalizing behaviour and externalizing behaviour of all three measurement waves are presented in Table 3. Table 4 shows

the correlations between all latent variables. The independence model testing the hypothesis that all cannabis scores and internalizing behaviour scores were uncorrelated was rejected: χ2 (30, N = 1,449) = 56.4, p < .003. The model provided an acceptable fit to

the data (CFI = .99, RMSEA = .03). However, as shown in Table 3, correlations between internalizing behaviour problems (T1-2-3) and cannabis use (T2-T3) ranged from .02 to .06 and thus are very small. Although these correlations were significant (probably due to the large sample size), they of were indicative of non-relationships between cannabis use and internalizing behaviour. This was confirmed by the Wald test. Dropping parameters indicative of associations between internalizing behaviour (T1, T2 and T3) and cannabis use (T2 and T3) resulted in a non-significant change of the model [χ2 (6, N = 1,449) = 11.2, p = .081]. Path-analysis revealed that although our model represented the data well [χ2 (66, N = 1,449) = 215.2, p < .001; RMSEA = .04, CFI = .97], all paths between internalizing (T1-2-3) and cannabis use (T2-T3) were non-significant. The independence model that tested the hypothesis that all cannabis scores and externalizing behaviour scores were uncorrelated, was rejected: χ2 (9, N = 1,449) = 64.4, p < .001. Also, although RMSEA was relatively high (.07), the CFI was .99 and therefore our model provided an acceptable fit to the data. Correlations between externalizing behaviour (T1-2-3) and cannabis use (T2-T3) ranged from .19 to .58 and thus were indicative of a relationship between externalizing behaviour problems and cannabis use (see Table 4).

To study the general anatomy of the monarch brain, we first perfo

To study the general anatomy of the monarch brain, we first performed find more three-dimensional

reconstructions of the major brain neuropils. We labeled whole-brain preparations of the monarch with antibodies against the synaptic marker Synapsin, acquired confocal image stacks, and, based on these images, reconstructed the size and shape of identifiable brain regions. The resulting architecture of the monarch brain (Figures 2A–2C) was similar to that of other insect brains, with close resemblance to the brain of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta, the only other lepidopteran brain that has been examined in comparable detail ( El Jundi et al., 2009). In the monarch optic lobes, we identified and reconstructed the medulla, the lobula, the lobula plate, and the accessory medulla. In the central brain, we identified and reconstructed the central complex (CC), the anterior optic tubercles (AOTu), the antennal lobes, and the mushroom bodies (Figures 2A–2C). Because the present work focused on sun compass neuropils, the mushroom body lobes were not separated into their components; higher image resolution is required to resolve their compressed and intertwined organization. The monarch AOTu, the first processing stage for sun compass information in the

central brain of the locust (Pfeiffer et al., 2005), can be divided into three components: the large upper division, the lower division, and the nodular division, with its distinct, glomerular organization (Figure 2D). With the AZD5363 staining procedure used,

the monarch LALs, the second relay stage for compass information in the locust brain, did not show distinct boundaries, consistent with the situation in the hawkmoth. Etomidate However, the monarch LALs were defined on both sides of the CC using single-cell morphologies (Figures 3C, 3E, and 3G). Positioned posterior of the antennal lobe, they extended anteriorly, approximately from the depth of the lower division of the central body (CBL), until they almost reached the anterior surface of the brain. Simultaneous dye-fills of multiple CBL-tangential neurons revealed a distinct neuropil region containing the postsynaptic endings of these cells (Figures 3A and 3B). Because of the typical, microglomerular shape of these endings, this neuropil region is probably the monarch homolog of the combined lateral triangle/medial olive areas of the locust (data not shown). Spatially, these areas were located on either side of the midline, between the LAL and the posterioventral surface of the mushroom body lobes, and slightly anterior to the CBL. We next focused on the detailed reconstruction of the monarch CC, the proposed integration site of sun compass information in the insect brain.