However, the strategy of designing viruses that can leverage the

However, the strategy of designing viruses that can leverage the large and rapidly growing armamentarium of animal lines that express exogenous recombinases only in defined cell types (driver lines, which can fully capitalize on enormous native promoter/enhancer regions rather than the small fragments which fit Erastin nmr into viruses) offers an expanded range of opsin targeting strategies (Figure 2B; see Table 3 for driver lines used in optogenetic studies). New driver lines

are continually added to the available repertoire by groups such as GENSAT and the Allen Institute for Brain Science. Successfully utilizing a recombinase driver line requires efficient packaging of the genetic material to be expressed into a recombinase-dependent system conferring the two properties of (1) very low leak (background) of opsin expression in non-recombinase-expressing cells, and (2) very high recombinase-induced opsin expression—all within the viral backbone. Several potential different recombinase-dependent viral vector designs have emerged (Kuhlman and Huang, 2008, Zhang, 2008, Atasoy et al., 2008 and Sohal et al., 2009), and a Cre recombinase-dependent double-floxed inverted opsin gene in AAV under the EF1α promoter (Zhang, 2008 and Sohal et al., 2009) or the CAG promoter

(Atasoy et al., 2008) was ultimately found to provide a suitable combination of strength and specificity to enable behaviorally significant optogenetic gain or loss of function within the constraints of the freely moving mammal system see more (Tsai et al., 2009 and Aponte et al., 2011). Not only is this strategy versatile in the sense that it can be applied at will to the large and growing pool of Cre driver lines (e.g., Gong et al., 2007), soon to include rat as well as mouse lines, but this approach is also by design expandable along new dimensions that enable combinatorial experiments (Figure 2). First, other recombinases such as Flp or Dre may be used to construct orthogonal driver lines that can be crossed with Cre driver lines while the same low-leak, high-potency recombinase-dependent AAV design

is theoretically adaptable for these other recombinases as well. Second, promoter fragments may be ADP ribosylation factor used at the same time in place of the EF1α promoter in the recombinase-dependent viruses, thereby implementing intersecting promoter and recombinase-dependent specificity. Third, while generation of recombinase-dependent opsin mouse lines for simply crossing with Cre driver lines is a viable approach (Madisen et al., 2010a, Madisen et al., 2010b, Kätzel et al., 2011 and Zariwala et al., 2011), resulting opsin expression levels may be weaker than with high-copy-number recombinase-dependent viruses, and more importantly the viral approach provides a unique advantage of intersecting genetic and anatomic specificity. To illustrate this point, consider that for most Cre driver lines, specificity exists only at particular points in space and time.

The need to include a number of components of fitness into the tr

The need to include a number of components of fitness into the training programmes of soccer players would indicate that the exercise prescription should be multi-dimensional. The inclusion of specific training plans for the development of a number of energy systems SKI-606 concentration as well as specific muscle exercises would lead to a need for multiple types of physical training sessions.

The completion of a large number of such training sessions is problematic in a sport such as soccer for various reasons. The need to include training that is focussed on the development/practice of technical skills and sessions that impact on the tactical requirements of soccer prevent the completion of numerous physical training sessions. Technical/tactical sessions are frequently the priority in the training plan and will therefore often take precedent

overall other training activities. The large number of competitive fixtures, as well as the need for frequent travel, further limits the time that is available to undertake physical training in the competitive season. These restrictions IOX1 cell line promote the need for a more global approach to the training of players by devising sessions that promote the simultaneous development of physical, technical, tactical, and mental qualities. The restrictive framework that governs the inclusion of sessions focussed on purely physical conditioning makes planning a priority. Detailed planning of both the acute and chronic physical training sessions ensures that training is efficient in its delivery. This will help to maximise the performance improvements associated

with the training completed by the players. This article aims to outline the theoretical approach used to plan physical 4-Aminobutyrate aminotransferase training in soccer. It also includes important information on the sport-specific way to deliver a physical training stimulus. A short section on the importance of monitoring the activity completed by players will also be included as such strategies are vital to performance, especially for the modern elite player. Periodisation is a theoretical model that offers a framework for the planning and systematic variation of an athlete’s training prescription.1 Periodisation was originally developed to support the training process in track and field or similar sports in which there is a clear overall objective such as training tailored towards a major championship such as the Olympics.2 The inclusion of variation in the prescribed training load is thought to be a fundamentally important concept in successful training programmes.3 This is a consequence of the sustained exposure to the same training load failing to elicit further adaptations. Sustained training loads, especially if they are high, can also lead to mal-adaptations such as fatigue and injury. Both these outcomes would result in ineffective training sessions and a failure to benefit performance of both the individual athlete and the team.

The coarse targeting specified by protein gradients can then be r

The coarse targeting specified by protein gradients can then be refined by additional mechanisms, such as class-specific labeling molecules (Hong

et al., 2009, Kaneko-Goto et al., 2008 and Serizawa et al., 2006), which further segregate the olfactory circuit into discrete glomeruli. A P-element insertion sema-2aP2 was used for all sema-2a loss-of-function analysis ( Ayoob et al., 2006). Two alleles of sema-2b were used—a pBac insertion f02042 ( Thibault et al., 2004) and FRT-mediated deletion #078 (Sema-2bC4) ( Wu et al., 2011). A P-element insertion KG00878 into PlexB was used for loss-of-function analysis ( Ayoob et al., 2006). UAS-sema-2a RNAi transformant ID #15811 was used for all RNAi experiments (VDRC). To HSP inhibitor label Mz19+ PN dendrites, Mz19-GAL4 or Mz19-mCD8GFP were used as previously described ( Berdnik et al., 2006). To label DL3 dendrites, the enhancer trap line HB5-43-GAL4 (kindly provided by U. Heberlein and screened by E.C. Marin) was used. Cilengitide order To label VM2 dendrites, NP5103-GAL4 was used as previously characterized ( Komiyama et al., 2007). Other transgenes were from the following sources: pebbled-GAL4 ( Sweeney et al., 2007); UAS-HA-PlexA ( Terman et al., 2002); UAS-PlexB ( Ayoob et al., 2006); UAS-Sema-2a-TM ( Wu et al., 2011);

UAS-Sema-2a ( Winberg et al., 1998a). MARCM analysis to label DL1 single cell clones was performed as previously described (Komiyama et al., 2003 and Lee and Luo, 1999). In summary, flies were heat shocked for

1 hr at 37°C between 2 and 26 hr after larval hatching. QMARCM labeled DL1 single cell clones (Potter et al., 2010) were similarly generated but were heat shocked for an additional hour to increase clone frequency. VM2 anterodorsal neuroblast clones were generated between 0, 24, 48, and 72 hr after larval hatching and clone frequency was increased by performing two 1-hr heat shocks with 30 min at room temperature in between heat shocks. Additionally, flies were raised at 18°C after heat shocking for 24 hr. Or83b-GAL4 ( Larsson et al., 2004), which is expressed in all larval ORNs and ∼70%–80% adult ORNs, was used to drive the expression out of diphtheria toxin (DTI) ( Han et al., 2000). To specifically ablate larval ORNs and leave adult ORNs intact, tub-GAL80ts ( McGuire et al., 2003) was used to suppress GAL4 expression after puparium formation. Animals were raised at 25°C for 2 hr to allow egg laying and then shifted to 29°C to inactive GAL80, thus allowing GAL4 and DTI expression to ablate all larval ORNs. Animals were shifted back to 18°C at 0 hr APF to activate GAL80 and to allow the survival of adult ORNs. Immunostaining was performed according to previously described methods (Sweeney et al., 2007). Mouse anti-Sema-2a (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank) was used at a concentration of 1:50.

This was quantified by measuring the span of the YFP signal in th

This was quantified by measuring the span of the YFP signal in the basal process at the time points immediately preceding and following the time when the tip of the basal process had extended to reach its final, and most basal, position. Pooling the data from three time points before contact and three time points after contact, a significant change in the span of the YFP signal within the basal process is observed (Figure 2H), where the length decreases from 10.6 ± 0.6 μm (mean ± SEM) before contact, to 6.1 ± 0.4 μm after contact (p < 0.0001: Mann-Whitney test, n = 7 cells from three embryos). Normalizing

the measured lengths to the longest length observed for each cell, and centering the data on basal surface contact (t = 0), the trend of decreasing Kif5c560-YFP Trametinib in vitro signal length immediately following basal surface contact is apparent (Figure 2I). This specific accumulation

remains until a Kif5c560-YFP-positive growth cone sprouts from the cell and extends toward the optic nerve head. The YFP signal remains accumulated in the growth cone throughout this extension, and is not visible in the remainder of the cell. Because the specific accumulation of Kif5c560-YFP at the tip of the basal process correlates, both in time and in space, with RGCs contacting the basal surface of the retina, we hypothesized that an extracellular cue localized to CH5424802 clinical trial this region plays a role in this event. The extracellular matrix component Lam1 is a heterotrimer consisting of three subunits (α1, β1, and γ1), and contact with Lam1 is known to be able to polarize neurons in vitro and promote axon growth in RGCs (Esch et al., 1999 and Ménager et al., 2004). Moreover, it has been shown that zebrafish embryos lacking the Lamα1 subunit display severe axon guidance defects in multiple neuronal types, including RGCs (Paulus and

Halloran, 2006). Using a polyclonal antibody raised (-)-p-Bromotetramisole Oxalate against Lam1, strong staining is seen at the basal lamina lining the surfaces of the zebrafish retina (Figure 3Ai) (Lee and Gross, 2007), making it a strong candidate for directing RGC polarization. To test the necessity for Lamα1 in the normal polarization of RGCs in vivo, we injected a previously described lamα1 morpholino ( Pollard et al., 2006) into ath5:GAP-GFP transgenic embryos. Morpholino injections generally resulted in a complete loss of the Lam1 staining at the ILM ( Figure 3Aii). Strong Lam1 staining remained at Bruch’s membrane at the apical retinal surface, indicating that other α chains could be compensating for the loss of Lamα1 in this region. However, because the RPE acts as a physical barrier between Bruch’s membrane and retinal neurons, for our purposes we can assume that the Lamα1-deficient retina is devoid of any accessible Lam1.

It is the underlying assumption of linearity in many feedforward

It is the underlying assumption of linearity in many feedforward models, then, that leads to the conclusion that inhibition is required to explain cross-orientation VEGFR inhibitor suppression. Contrast saturation and response rectification, however, are highly nonlinear. For the test + mask stimulus, the responses of the LGN cells that see no contrast modulation necessarily still

fall to zero. But the responses of those LGN cells that see twice the contrast modulation (e.g., Figure 2H, red neuron) do not double. Their response to the test stimulus itself was already near saturation, so doubling the stimulus contrast increases the cell’s responses only slightly. As a result, the sum of the LGN responses—and therefore the input to the simple cell—falls in the presence of the mask (compare Figures 2I and 2J). Introducing realistic contrast saturation and rectification to an otherwise linear feedforward model results in cross-orientation suppression that is almost identical to that observed in real simple cells. In the model, the depolarization in a simple cell was taken to be proportional to the summed responses of eight LGN cells whose receptive fields were aligned in space. Response saturation and rectification were inserted by drawing the LGN responses from

a database of the recorded responses of cat LGN cells (Priebe and Ferster, 2006). Cross-orientation suppression in the model matched closely the suppression observed in the Vm responses of V1 simple cells: 9% for the high-contrast test gratings and 52% for low-contrast test gratings (Priebe and Ferster,

2006). ZD1839 datasheet To calculate the corresponding cross-orientation suppression in the spike responses of the model cell, the depolarization Thiamine-diphosphate kinase evoked by each stimulus was raised to the third power, to simulate the expansive nonlinearity of threshold as smoothed by trial-to-trial variability. The resulting cross-orientation suppression of the model’s spiking responses (29% and 89% for high- and low-contrast test stimuli) is consequently larger than what is predicted for Vm and is comparable to what has been observed experimentally. While nonlinearities in relay cell responses account for the mask-induced reduction in the modulation component of simple cell Vm, these nonlinearities also predict a rise in the mean LGN input to V1 neurons, which is not observed experimentally. This discrepancy might arise in part from synaptic depression at the thalamocortical synapse (Carandini et al., 2002 and Freeman et al., 2002) and because many simple cells receive less than half of their excitatory input from the LGN (Chung and Ferster, 1998 and Ferster et al., 1996). In addition, some of the predictions of this model appear at odds with the interactions between low-contrast test + mask, for which relay cell contrast saturation should have little effect but nonetheless have been reported to interact in cortical complex cells (Busse et al.

, 2006) prior to experimentation This manipulation failed to eli

, 2006) prior to experimentation. This manipulation failed to elicit LTPGABA in response to HFS (119% ± 11.5% of baseline, n = 8, p = 0.284; Figure 6C).

These data suggest that prolonged activation of the HPA axis (∼24 hr) is required to shift synapses from a depressing to a potentiating state. One additional prolonged stressor may result from animals being housed individually during the food deprivation period. To rule out the possibility that social isolation alone (as a mild stressor) is sufficient to unmask LTPGABA, we investigated whether HFS would elicit LTPGABA in animals housed alone but given ad libitum access to food for 24 hr prior to slice preparation. Under these conditions, synapses did not exhibit LTPGABA, but instead underwent an activity-dependent depression in GABA find more transmission (71% ± 12.2% of baseline, n = 5, p = 0.032; Figure 6C), indicating that social isolation is not sufficient to shift the polarity of the plasticity. Finally, we asked whether these synaptic changes could be reversed by the re-introduction of food. Following food deprivation, animals were given unlimited

access to food for 24 hr and then slices containing the DMH were prepared for electrophysiology. Refeeding following food deprivation restores circulating CORT to basal levels within 6 hr of food presentation (Jahng et al., 2005). Following refeeding, HFS did not elicit LTPGABA (88% ± 12.7% of baseline, n = 5, www.selleckchem.com/products/VX-770.html p = 0.919; Figure 6C), potentially

suggesting partial functional recovery of CB1Rs. In agreement with this finding, WIN 55,212-2–induced depression of GABA synapses was restored after 24 hr of refeeding (63% ± 11.0% of baseline, n = 4, p = 0.021). Taken together, these results indicate that a food deprivation–induced rise in CORT leads to a downregulation PD184352 (CI-1040) of CB1Rs, thus creating a permissive state that favors the induction of LTPGABA (Figure 7). The data presented here demonstrate that the feeding state of an animal determines the polarity of plasticity exhibited by GABA synapses in the DMH in response to repetitive synaptic stimuli. In satiated animals, GABA synapses undergo eCB-mediated LTD that requires NO. Following acute food deprivation, however, only LTP is evident. LTPGABA, which requires the heterosynaptic activation of NMDARs, is constrained in satiated animals by eCBs. Blockade of CB1Rs or their down-regulation during food deprivation by circulating CORT biases the synapses toward LTP. These findings provide, to the best of our knowledge, the first demonstration of state-dependent plasticity of a feeding circuit in response to acute food deprivation, and highlight a complex interaction between retrograde signals in which NO is necessary for LTD, and eCBs gate LTPGABA.

Extensive sellec

Extensive http://www.selleckchem.com/B-Raf.html projections to the basal ganglia from nearly all parts of the cerebral cortex led early neuroscientists to suggest that these subcortical structures are “the seat of the ‘sensorium commune’” (Thomas Willis, 1667, as cited in Wilson, 1914) and that “the royal road of the sensations of the body to the soul is through the corpora striata [the primary input to the basal ganglia] and all determinations of the will also descend by that road” (Emanuel Swedenborg, 1740, as cited in Wilson, 1914). However, this

focus on sensation was overshadowed by discoveries that disturbances in the basal ganglia cause muscle contraction and movement disorders, leading to extensive studies of their roles in the selection, initiation, and execution of voluntary movements (Denny-Brown, 1962, Ferrier, 1873 and Wilson, 1914). This work, in turn, has provided a framework for recent re-examinations of the basal ganglia’s contributions to nonmotor functions (reviewed in Brown et al., 1997). For example, it has been suggested that the parallel anatomical loops within the basal

ganglia pathway provide a “centralized selection mechanism” that resolves conflicts at multiple levels and in different domains, thus allowing the basal ganglia to “play a comparable role in cognition to that of action selection in motor control” (Redgrave et al., 1999). In this learn more Perspective, we tie these ideas about Electron transport chain the basal ganglia’s role in motor control and cognition back to perception, summarizing recent work that implicates this subcortical circuit in specific computations used to form perceptual

decisions. Perceptual decisions are categorical judgments about the presence or identity of sensory stimuli. Examples include determining whether or not a car is approaching, identifying a face in a crowd, or detecting a faint cry for help. These kinds of decisions are not reflexive responses to sensory input but rather deliberative processes that combine the available sensory evidence with information related to the known alternatives, past experiences, goals, and other factors to reach a categorical judgment that can guide behavior. These deliberative processes contribute to all measurable aspects of perception, from sensitivity to low-level features of sensory input to the ability to parse complex scenes (Gold and Ding, 2013 and Parker and Newsome, 1998). The underlying neural mechanisms include contributions from many parts of the cerebral cortex, particularly integrative areas in the parietal and prefrontal cortices (Gold and Shadlen, 2007 and Heekeren et al., 2008). However, the complex computations needed to implement these processes and use them to guide behavior are not solely a cortical phenomenon.

This same phenomenon was detected in WT neurons 20–24 hr after th

This same phenomenon was detected in WT neurons 20–24 hr after the addition of bicuculline (Figure 7C and 7D), which parallels the time course of Homer1a protein induction (Figure 5A). Acute Bay and MPEP did not alter mEPSCs in WT neurons after 48 hr treatment of bicuculline (Figures S6A and S6B). These data suggest that Homer1a contributes to the induction of homeostatic scaling by enhancing mGluR activity, and this mechanism makes relatively less contribution to maintenance of scaling.

In further support of this model, Bay and MPEP treatment, selleck chemical which blocks the scaling effect of bicuculline (Figure 1), does not reverse bicuculline scaling even if applied for an additional 48 hr after bicuculline (Figures S6C and S6D). To examine Homer 1a scaling in vivo, we monitored responses of layer II-III pyramidal neurons in the acute cortical slices. As in culture, Homer1a KO pyramidal neurons had larger amplitude mEPSCs than

WT neurons (Figures S7A and S7B: Homer1a KO 13.2 ± 0.8 pA; n = 7 cells; WT 10.4 ± 0.7 pA; n = 8 cells; ∗p < 0.05). There was no difference in mEPSC frequency between WT (14.9 ± 1.7 Hz; n = 8 cells) and Homer1a KO neurons (16.7 ± 3.8 Hz; n = 7 cells; Figures S7A and S7B). Acute application of Bay (50 μM) and MPEP (10 μM) to slices from naive WT or Homer1a KO mice did not change the amplitude of mEPSCs (not shown). To assess the contribution of activity-inducible Homer1a, we treated WT and Homer1a KO mice with MECS and prepared cortical slices 2 hr later. Slices were

used for recordings 1–2 hr after preparation to correspond to the point of maximal expression of Homer1a protein after MECS (Brakeman et al., see more 1997). Homer1a mRNA was detected by in situ hybridization in ∼13% of layer II-III cortical neurons in naive mice, and in ∼35% of neurons in MECS treated mice (Figures S7C and S7D). Accordingly, we anticipated that effects of Homer1a might be evident in approximately one-third of randomly selected neurons. A comparison of mEPSC amplitudes in neurons from naive WT mice versus those treated with MECS showed a significant decrease in mEPSC amplitudes, whereas a similar comparison in Homer1a KO mice did not (Figure S7E). Bath application of Bay and MPEP increased the amplitude of mEPSCs in a subset of WT neurons (4/15) after MECS (Figures 7E and 7F). By contrast, bath application of Bay and MPEP aminophylline did not result in an increase in mEPSC amplitude of neurons from Homer1a KO mice (0 of 16 neurons; different from WT neurons p < 0.05 using Fisher’s exact test) (Figures 7E and 7F). To confirm the hypothesis that activity evokes constitutive mGluR signaling that reduces synaptic strength in WT mice, we employed a fos-GFP reporter mouse to identify living neurons that were activated by MECS (Barth et al., 2004). C-fos and Homer1a are coordinately induced in neurons by MECS ( Barnes et al., 1994), and GFP was detected in approximately one-third of layer II-III pyramidal neurons after MECS.

7 ± 2 4 years old) The graphs were formed using methods consiste

7 ± 2.4 years old). The graphs were formed using methods consistent with the previous literature, and the relationship between community size and node strength was quantified for both graphs. Figure 2A MG-132 research buy shows the correlation matrix that defines a graph formed of 264 putative areas (Power et al., 2011), the communities found within this graph, the sizes of these communities, and node strength at multiple thresholds. Linear fits of strength to community size are plotted. There is

an evident relation between community size and node strength. Similar analyses performed in a voxelwise network in the same data set are shown in Figure 2B. In the voxelwise network the relationship between community size and node strength is considerably stronger. Because there is no “correct” threshold at which to analyze a graph, these analyses were performed at many thresholds (those used in Power et al., 2011). Across thresholds, community size explained 11% ± 4% of the variance in strength in the areal network and 34% ± 5% of the variance in strength in the voxelwise network. It is possible that strong relationships

Ribociclib between strength and community size are actually typical of real-world networks. To investigate this possibility, 17 other real-world data sets (3 correlation, 14 noncorrelation) were analyzed in the manner just described (see the Experimental Procedures, Figure 3, and Figure S1, online, for sources and details of the networks). Strong relationships between strength and community size were observed in real-world correlation networks but were generally absent in real-world noncorrelation networks, consistent with the theoretical considerations outlined above. If the meaning of degree is confounded by community size in correlation until networks, one might wonder whether important nodes could still be identified as nodes with high degree relative to other nodes within their community. Guimera and Amaral have proposed a widely used classification scheme to identify node roles based on such a framework (Guimerà

and Nunes Amaral, 2005). Their approach uses two measures to characterize nodes: within-module degree Z score and participation coefficient (Figure 4A). Within-module degree Z score is the Z score of a node’s within-module degree; Z scores greater than 2.5 denote hub status. Participation coefficients measure the distribution of a node’s edges among the communities of a graph. If a node’s edges are entirely restricted to its community, its participation coefficient is 0. If the node’s edges are evenly distributed among all communities, the participation coefficient is a maximal value that approaches 1 (the maximal value depends on the number of communities present). Hubs with low participation coefficients are called “provincial” hubs because their edges are not distributed widely among communities, whereas hubs with higher participation coefficients are called “connector” hubs.

However, unlike in the locust brain, the orientation of the monar

However, unlike in the locust brain, the orientation of the monarch CC and the remaining central brain were rotated by roughly 90° (around the sagittal axis), with respect to the fixed orientation of the optic lobes. Therefore, the monarch CX-5461 clinical trial PB is located on the posterior side of the central body close to the posterior surface of the brain (Figures 2E–2G). The distinctly noncontinuous layout of two hemispheres of the monarch PB again resembled the situation in the hawkmoth. The central body and the PB were separated by a midline-spanning neuropil that left

two gaps on either side for passage of the W-, X-, Y-, and Z-bundles. These massive tracts provide the only connection between the PB and central body and contain the axons of all compass-related columnar neurons of the CC. Within the central body, the bean-shaped CBL was located anterior to the larger CBU, while the noduli were located ventrally (Figure 2E). We reconstructed individual neurons of the monarch CC to further define the butterfly sun compass network and substantiate that the layout of this brain area is highly conserved between locusts Smad inhibitor and monarchs. Of the five groups of neuron (TL, CL1, TB, CPU1, CP) known to constitute the polarization vision

network of the locust central complex (Heinze et al., 2009), we identified four groups in the monarch CC, covering all processing stages from the proposed input to the proposed output neurons of the CC (Figure 3; Table 1). Homologies were established based on detailed information regarding the location of input/output

arborizations, the structure of terminals, and the heterolateral connectivity patterns (Figure S1 available online, Figure 3, Table 1). Consequently, we classified the identified neurons as monarch butterfly versions of TL2 (n = 6), TL3 (n = 5), CL1 (n = 4), TB1 (n = 4), CPU1a (n = Dipeptidyl peptidase 6), and CPU1b neurons (n = 1). Although subtypes of CL1 neurons could not be identified unambiguously in the monarch, CPU1 subtypes (CPU1a/b) could be clearly distinguished in monarchs because all arborizations of CPU1b neurons were located contralateral to the soma (Figure 3E), which is the defining feature of locust CPU1b cells (Heinze and Homberg, 2008; Figure S1E). In addition to neurons of the core polarization-sensitive network, conditionally polarization-sensitive neurons of the locust provide another level of complexity to the CC-network, as they are thought to be recruited only in a context-dependent manner (Heinze and Homberg, 2009). Likewise, homologs of all three conditionally polarization-sensitive cell types were also identified in the monarch CC (CL2, CPU2, and CPU4) (Table 1). The only major types of neuron that were not found in the monarch were homologs of locust cells directly connecting the PB with the lateral triangle/medial olive (CP1, CP2).